Where the Wild Ones Are
Mike Schindlinger
First appeared in "Original Flying Machine" magazine
(copyright 2000, 2001)

 

In the mid 1970s, my father purchased a parrot from a Brooklyn pet store, without much though about where the bird originated. Charlie (as the Yellow-Crowned amazon, Amazona ochrocephala, was soon named) differed from most of the pet parrots I’d seen in one particularly striking way: he produced an abundance of highly complex sounds, compared to the much simpler sounds produced by captive-reared parrots - or those taken from wild nests as chicks, before they have had a chance to learn the sounds produced by members of their species in their natal environment. I have since come to understand that he brought these complex sounds with him on his long journey from the wild. Additionally, Charlie’s left wing did not have, and subsequently failed to regenerate, primary flight feathers. I learned that this condition is not unusual for birds caught from the wild as free-flying adults or subadults. Often the poachers will clip or cut the primaries upon capture, and any damage to the skin of the wing can inhibit subsequent feather growth. Or, birds whose feathers are not cut as such may damage them during the stressful early days of captivity by flapping their wings against the bars of a confining cage. I always knew that Charlie was quite out of place in New York City, but any hopes of someday returning him to his natal population were dimmed by his lack of these feathers, so critical for controlled flight.

Years later as a student in my first year of graduate school, while on a Tropical Ecology course field trip to Venezuela, I had my first opportunity to see parrots in the wild in a lowland rainforest area, Tico Poro. As luck would have it, they were Yellow-Crowned amazons gathered in a fruiting tree just after dawn, behind a small roadside restaurant, and I watched and listened raptly, while my classmates slept back at camp. Though the Yellow-Crowns made sounds which were somewhat different from those made by Charlie, the quality of their voices seemed nonetheless quite familiar to me, and I felt a sense of kinship with these wild birds, engendered by my close relationship with Charlie. I was also struck by the contrast of their flock-based society with the much more solitary lives of the captive parrots I had seen, the space of whose lives are often constricted to the volume of their cage, or at most, to the volume of the homes of their owners. What had always been a sense of injustice about whatever events has transpired to remove Charlie from such a flock and ship him to New York to become a captive apartment dweller burgeoned within me into a sense of obligation. Though I could never return him to the wild, perhaps I could do something to help ensure that other wild parrots, living free among their own, might not suffer the fate of his own dislocation.

When I returned from Venezuela, I was curious about how Charlie would respond to the recording I had made of the wild flock; if I could not bring him to the wild, perhaps I could bring the wild to him. In the past I had played his own recorded vocalizations back to him, and he was always stimulated to sing along, or even to circle the speaker avidly, apparently in search of the parrot singing within. I anticipated that the recording I was about to play to him would elicit an even more dramatic response, as it was from numerous individuals of his species. But quite to my amazement (and dismay), as I started playing the recording for him, he continued to preen himself disinterestedly. Whatever interest he did express was no different from his response to any loud sounds, and certainly came nowhere near the reaction elicited by his own voice, his own sounds. I subsequently had the chance to play for Charlie recordings of his species that I made in Peru, and the effect upon him was the same: indifference.

It occurred to me that the differences between his wild-type sounds and those of the population I had recorded, though perhaps subtle to human ears, may have been profound enough in his ears to preclude any sense of recognition Charlie might have otherwise had, if the recording had come from within his natal population. The body of work I undertook in my graduate studies, on a species closely related to Charlie’s, was born out of this initial conjecture about the importance of the precise form of a parrot’s calls for recognition. If a close match between the stimulus vocalizations and the respondent’s own was indeed necessary to elicit the level of response I had seen in Charlie reacting to his own vocalizations, how would this shape the learning strategy of a young parrot? How would such social discrimination shape the differences in vocal repertoires among individuals within a population? Among populations within a species? Among species?

These are the questions which formed the conceptual basis of the work I conducted as a Ph.D. candidate at Harvard … in a nutshell.

A Closely Related Species: The Yellow-Headed Amazon

The Yellow-Headed amazon parrot, Amazona oratrix, is one of the premier talkers of the parrots. Their scientific species name “oratrix” is Latin for “she who speaks.” They have a very long lifespan, rivaling our own, and form long-term pair-bonds with their mates. They are also highly prized by the pet-trade ($800 to $1,500 in New York City), and so wild populations are in peril, as they face the combined threats of deforestation (they nest in old, hollow trees) and capture for the pet trade. The profits from parrot smuggling rival those for cocaine, pound for pound. Thus, there is real incentive for people from poorer countries to poach nests and send the fledglings northward.

Some Observations from the Wild

Within an area, each pair of parrots (or occasionally a trio) has a stable home range from year to year, and all of the oratrix parrots in a given area share a common “lexicon” of sounds. These sounds, though learned, appear to be highly stable through time and across a wide geographic area. When I traveled to a location 60 miles north of my main study area, I found that the parrots there were using the same vocabulary, or vocal repertoire, as the parrots further south, though there were some noticeable differences. The northern parrots speak with a different “accent,” equivalent perhaps to a “northern drawl.” The similarities, though, strongly imply that the two sites are not isolated from each other, but form one continuous population. This conclusion was given more credence when I recorded a juvenile at the main field site that had the northern accent, indicating that this juvenile was an immigrant who had come from the northern area.

At another site, about 80 miles inland from my main study area, I found a population that possessed a vocal repertoire which was entirely different from the coastal populations. This difference was not like an “accent” when using the same words. I could not find equivalent sounds. It is more like differences among human languages or musical traditions. This seems to imply that this inland population has long been isolated from the others, at least long enough that they would form their own “oral traditions.”

I then performed a crucial experiment back at the main field site. Using tape recorders and loud speaker-horns, I played back recordings made at each of the three sites to see how the parrots would respond to the differences. They responded (by vocalizing and approaching) to tapes from their own and the similar-sounding population, but hardly, if at all, to the tapes from the different-sounding population, as if they did not recognize that it was from the same species. I interpret these results as follows: If a parrot spends a lot of time and energy vocalizing, it is doing so to get and to hold the attention of the other parrots in an area (“Over here! Look at me!”). If the listening parrot ignores sounds which are very different from those it is familiar with, then there would be a strong social pressure for the singing parrot to conform to the local dialect in order to get the attention it seeks (and we all know how parrots are big hams when it comes to getting attention). Thus, parrots which are better imitators can get more attention from their intended audience.

At another site, I made some recordings that I then compared (using a computer) to a recording that was made in the same vicinity in 1961 (which I got from the Cornell Library of Natural Sounds). Much to my amazement, some of these 1961 sounds were still being used 34 years later. So, it seems, these parrots are rather conservative when it comes to changing their vocal repertoire.

Also, for these parrots, vocal imitation is limited to members of their own species. Though there were three species of amazon parrots at my main field site, there was no evidence that they were imitating each other, or any other species for that matter. This is in direct contrast to the African Grey parrot, also a renowned talker, which has been documented to imitate the sounds of other species in the wild. So it appears that whatever social factors are at work in shaping learned vocal repertoires in the “talking” parrots, there are different forces in play in different species.

What’s All the Chatter About?

After standing in the field with them month after month, then year after year, I began to see that many of their vocalizations have a clearly contextual function, such as coordinating a pair’s movements when they are about to fly off (“c’mon, let’s go!”), or to keep a pair in contact when they separate (“over here!”). The vast majority of their complex sounds, however, may have more in common with music than with speech. By this I mean that there are regularly repeated patterns, and often seem to be given in more of a one-sided performance than an interactive dialog. How can we ask what a parrot “means” by these vocalizations? Let me draw an analogy: When we hear a saxophonist playing his solo, what does he say through the notes he chooses? Well, he’s telling us about his stylistic background (Is he a jazz musician? A blues man? Classical?); about his proficiency (Is he a musical genius, or just a journeyman? Is he just starting out, or an old pro?). He may also be making references to other bits of music from within his genre (as when we recognize a snatch of melody from another song go by), but these are only apparent if we, the audience, share his stylistic background and recognize these musical references and quotes. Thus, though he may not be speaking with words, the saxophonist is actually communicating quite a bit about himself. These same kinds of information may be communicated by singing parrots (where they come from, how proficient they are, how old they are, how vast their recollection of sounds is, etc.).

Of course, if the parrots were speaking about last week’s weather, how would we know?

Some Potential Applications of My Results

The stability of parrot vocalizations within an area through time means that we can answer questions about whether populations are connected (as when I recorded a juvenile from the northern site in my main field area). We can even learn from where confiscated parrots are being poached (I think again about Charlie, and someday finding a population where they all speak the same “language” as him). For example, tapes I got from a woman in Wisconsin indicate that her bird was wild-caught from the northern population. I have since heard from others who have heard the recordings on my web site, which match the vocalizations that their own companion parrots make. Hopefully, we can use this information to target law enforcement and education campaigns to those areas from which parrots in the wild are still being harvested illegally.

Also, with the production of my film on the lives of free-living parrots, “Stalking the Wild Amazons,” I hope to help replace the popular perception of parrots as cute little caged clowns (as in the movie “Paulie,” which no doubt created a run on parrots as pets, as did “101 Dalmatians” for those dogs) with a more realistic, and hence more sympathetic, view of wild parrots. Hopefully we can redirect some of our love and appreciation for parrots away from some of the potentially destructive aspects of the pet trade and into a more conservation-minded perspective, eco-tourism to where they still fly free, and support for wild parrot conservation efforts.

Lastly, by expanding the broad front of research on animal communication to include parrots, we may come to know more about the origins and evolution of that aspect of ourselves which makes us uniquely human: our language. We just might discover that we are not as unique as we have thought, and that the bright light of our intelligence is, in fact, visible as a spark of cunning in numerous other species as well. Perhaps evolution will eventually fan this spark into a bright flame, if we can preserve these species long enough for it to do so.

For more information about my work and the fascinating lives of parrots in the wild, or to obtain a copy of “Stalking the Wild Amazons,” a one-hour documentary on the lives of wild parrots, please visit my website: http://surf.to/parrots, or write to me at 23 Sacramento Place, Cambridge, MA 02138.